January 8, 2021
The Evaluator as Moral Fiduciary
Ernest R. House
I
have long advocated the idea of justice in evaluation and, in particular, the
deliberative democratic approach. As society changes, we might add new
techniques and practices to meet new challenges and strengthen our approach.
What might these challenges be, and what should we do to meet them?
Evaluations don’t
seem as effective as they once were. Evaluator Tom Schwandt (2019) contends
that evaluation has entered a post-normal phase similar to that of post normal
science. In normal science, scientific studies are widely accepted by the
government and public and often acted upon. In post-normal times, findings are frequently
disputed, criticized, and ignored. Environmental science is an example, as is the
epidemiology of Covid-19.
I
believe these changes in evaluation and science reflect changes in the larger society.
Over decades, American society has become increasingly fragmented, polarized,
distrustful, unequal, and corrupt (House, 2020). In such a society, evaluations
become less effective. How can we make them more effective? Evaluators might act
as moral fiduciaries, cultivate cognitive empathy, focus on deep stories and
deep values, control for biases, and be transparent. In this chapter, I outline
significant changes in American society, delineate several practices that can
strength evaluations, discuss conditions of their implementation, and briefly tie
these practices to evaluation theory.
Societal Changes
Over the past
seventy years, American society has changed dramatically. When I was young, say
about 1960, there was a consensus among the majority that the government was
good and that society was on the right track. This consensus emerged from World
War II and Roosevelt’s New Deal (Kennedy, 1999). There was a palpable sense of
unity and national purpose. During the 1960s, the consensus and trust in
government began breaking down. There was the Vietnam War and the Civil Rights
movement, followed by Nixon and Watergate. These events engendered discord and distrust.
By 1980 Reagan could
run on the slogan, “The government is not the solution to the problem; the
government is the problem.” He initiated an era of privatization, deregulation,
and strong anti-government sentiment. In the new century, there was the
contested Bush/Gore election, the trauma of Nine Eleven, the Afghan and Iraq
wars, and the Great Financial Crisis. Distrust deepened. American society
fragmented.
Three powerful
trends emerged: a deep decline in trust, a sharp rise in inequality, and an
increase in corruption. People did not trust the government, they did not trust
their institutions, and they did not trust each other. Decline in trust was
precipitous during Vietnam and Watergate when trust in government dropped from
77% in 1964 to 30% by 1980 (Rothstein, 2018). By 2014 trust in government was
20%. Trust in Congress was 6%. Not all countries are like this. Trust in
government in the Nordic countries is 70%.
Another powerful
trend has been steepening inequality. In 1981 Reagan lowered the tax rate on
the wealthy, followed by more tax cuts by later administrations. America now
has the most extreme inequality of income and wealth of any developed country (Saez and Zucman, 2019, Stiglitz, 2018, Piketty, 2014). The
three richest Americans own more than the bottom 160 million. The richest .1
percent have as much wealth as the lower 90 percent (Hacker and Pierson, 2020,
Krugman, 2017). CEOs of major corporations used to make 20 times the average
worker wage. They now make 360 times as much (Dubchuk
and Fried, 2004). Top hedge fund managers make 100 million dollars per month (Saez and Zucman, 2019). Worker wages have stagnated for
decades. Social mobility has declined. Nobel economist Joseph Stiglitz (2018)
said the economy is rigged in favor of the wealthy. Conservative media mogul
Rupert Murdoch noted that America is developing permanent social classes.
A third trend has
been worsening corruption. When Governor Blagojevich of Illinois tried to fill
the Senate seat vacated by Obama, few expected him to auction the seat to the
highest bidder for personal gain. Patronage had become corruption. American
society was beset with cronyism, favoritism, and conflicts of interest. Special
interests dominated Congress and legislatures to secure special favors (Lessig,
2018). Seventy-five percent of Americans believe corruption is widespread
throughout the government (Rothstein 2018). Political scientists call such
behavior institutional corruption
Distrust,
inequality, and corruption interact in downward spirals. The more a government
is distrusted, the less it can remedy corruption or inequality, and the more it
becomes vulnerable to cronyism and conflicts of interest. Distrust allows
corruption that generates more distrust and inequality. These trends are
reflected in dystopian novels, apocalyptic films, and polarized politics (Hacker
and Pierson, 2020).
Act as a Moral Fiduciary
How
should evaluators behave in such a society? Problems in the larger society are manifested
in evaluation. Evaluators encounter fragmentation, distrust, inequality, and corruption,
such as conflicts of interest, in their work. They are often mistrusted and work
with stakeholders with quite diverse views. Some sources of inequality, such as
tax policies, lie beyond evaluation, but evaluators can attend to inequalities,
fragmentation, distrust, and corruption within the evaluation space. They can
act as moral fiduciaries. They might assume some moral fiduciary responsibility
for helping those lower in the unequal socio-economic structure. They can
strive to protect the interests of those less advantaged. Here’s an example.
In 2009 eleven
thousand forensic rape kits were discovered in a police warehouse in Detroit.
The kits had not been processed. For the victims, undergoing a rape
investigation is invasive. The process takes hours and involves swabbing
samples from every orifice of the body. Rape is a crime often committed by repeat
offenders, and rape kits provide an opportunity to identify serial rapists. Yet
the kits remained unexamined for years.
In an examination
of police files, Rebecca Campbell and her colleagues (Campbell, Shaw, and Fehler-Cabral, 2015) discovered that the police
investigating the crimes repeatedly dismissed victim claims of rape on the
grounds that the women were prostitutes, sexually permissive, or did not want
their parents to know they had sexual partners. In the judgment of the police,
the women’s claims were not worth pursuing as crimes. Such judgments reflected police
racial framing about the presumed character of minority women. Police reports
often referred to victims in highly pejorative terms.
Campbell and her
colleagues worked through the cases with police until the rape kits were
processed, an exhausting exercise, with police officials admitting that
classifying women this way was mistaken. In a sense, the researchers appealed
to empirical evidence and the police sense of fairness. This was a difficult
study to conduct because it involved conflicts of deep stories and deep values.
During the study, investigators kept in mind the welfare of those who had been
abused. Their welfare was most at stake, not that of the police nor those who
funded the study. In such conditions of inequality, evaluators might give
priority to the interests of those less able to defend their interests.
What is a
fiduciary? In the financial community, the idea of a fiduciary means that the
financial agent must act in the best interests of the client. As an experienced
investor, I can attest that investment transactions are riven with conflicts of
interest. For example, if an investor asks the advice of a financial advisor,
the advisor is free to recommend mutual funds that enrich the advisor, not the
client. Advisors often receive fees or a portion of the investment from mutual
funds they recommend. Clients suffer because this is not the best investment
for them. This is standard practice in finance.
Fortunately,
there are advisors who swear to a higher fiduciary ethical standard. That is,
they will offer advice that is in the best interests of the client. If they
fail, they can be sued. Reformers have tried to make the fiduciary ethic the
legal standard for investment professionals, but professionals have fought
vigorously against this, an indication of how prevalent not acting in the best
interests of the client is. Nonetheless, some advisors do choose to act as financial
fiduciaries.
Evaluators might
act as moral fiduciaries, meaning they act in the best interests of those less
able to defend their interests, rather than in the interests of the sponsors or
other stakeholders. Such an ethic doesn’t preclude acting in other stakeholder
interests as well, but it does mean those less advantaged will be given
priority. In most programs, it’s not difficult to determine who’s less
advantaged. In Detroit, Campbell and her colleagues chose to work through case
records with police to process the kits. They did not seek to reform the police
view overall, but to change their view about specific cases. Evaluators can
take some responsibility for the severe inequality in their society by doing
something about it in actual evaluations.
Cultivate Cognitive Empathy
In conducting the
Detroit study, the evaluators exercised some complex skills. One was to
understand what the police were thinking. That insight was key to figuring out
why the rape kits had not been processed. In his analysis, Schwandt recommends
that evaluators strive to understand other people’s perspectives. Evaluators
cannot assume their perspective is the only perspective. This is critical in a
fragmented society.
Sociologist
Mario Small (2019) calls such understanding “cognitive empathy.” He contends
that social researchers are quantitatively literate, but not as qualitatively
literate. Qualitative literacy entails understanding other perspectives in
depth. Cognitive empathy is not feeling empathy. It’s not feeling the same as
others. Nor is it sympathy, feeling sorrow or pity for them. Rather it’s the
ability to understand people’s predicament as they understand it. Their view
will seem rational within their perspective. We can understand why they believe
the way they do. That doesn’t mean we agree with them. Certainly, Campbell and
her colleagues did not agree with the police stereotyping of minority women.
Small cautions
against overgeneralizing other views. He calls this avoiding “out-group
homogeneity bias.” When groups are far removed from our perspective, their
views appear less diverse than they are. Not all police are alike. Another
caution is to be sensitive to data used as supporting evidence. For example,
when journalists and others report standardized test scores, they often explain
the results based on no empirical evidence whatsoever. They may contend scores
are low because the education system is dysfunctional. But there’s no
supporting evidence about causes in the test scores results. Where do such
explanations come from? From beliefs and stereotypes held by those making the
claims.
Focus on Deep Stories
Deep stories are
the subjective prisms through which we view the world, including how we feel
about it. They are stories that “feel right” emotionally. They are
interpretations of events and situations that people act on. The idea of deep
stories comes from sociologist Arlie Hochschild (2016), who studied Tea Party voters
in Lake Charles, Louisiana. They voted for Trump overwhelmingly in 2016. In
their view, life is a long march towards the American dream, which lies just
over the hill. But the line they’re in is stalled. Others are cutting in line
ahead of them, minorities and immigrants, people who used to be behind them.
That’s not fair, in their view. Government agencies are helping these people.
It’s the government’s fault. Trump also blames the government, minorities, and
immigrants. He derides the elites. This is the “deep story” of the Tea Party.
It’s also the deep story of Fox News, their source of news. Fairness is
critical, construed a particular way.
Similarly, sociologist
Katherine Cramer (2016) studied people in small towns and rural areas of
Wisconsin. Their towns are struggling, though people work hard. They believe
the government is taking money away from them and giving it to minorities and
immigrants in the big cities. In their view, decision-makers don’t respect
rural people. These resentful voters tipped the state to Trump in 2016. People
act on their deep stories.
There are other
deep stories, like that of the progressives in Berkeley. In their view,
Americans built a magnificent public square, but marauders invaded the square,
dismantled it, and stole pieces to build private mansions. Massive
accumulations of wealth threaten democracy itself. That’s not fair, either.
They supported Bernie Sanders (Hochschild, 2016). These are a few of America’s
deep stories.
If I were
evaluating an environmental education program in these communities, it would
help considerably to understand their deep stories. Lake Charles has oil
refineries and toxic industries. Even though people die of cancer at high
rates, the residents see these industries as vital to their livelihoods and
have ways of thinking about the problem, including religion. In rural Wisconsin
my evaluation would be different, and different yet again in Berkeley.
Concentrate on Deep Values
Deep values are
entwined in deep stories. The Louisiana story is built around deep values about
race, gender, and social class, around assumptions of a hierarchy of race,
gender, and social class. It’s assumed that some should be behind others in the
natural order, a vision derived in part from Louisiana history, with its
tradition of populist movements led by politicians like Huey Long (Hochschild,
2016). Within this perspective, social status is determined by how far a person
is from the bottom. Being ahead of minorities and immigrants is a matter of
entitlement.
Everyone has a
hierarchy of values, with some values more central to the belief structure.
These might be called deep values or core values. For most, they include
family, fairness, and in-group loyalty. For some, they also include racism and
sexism, deep-seated beliefs about who belongs where and deserves what. Not all
deep values are good or benign. Everyone has a deep story and deep values, and
it’s worthwhile for evaluators to reflect on theirs. I’ve discussed mine elsewhere,
which began in childhood as a Roosevelt Democrat (House, 2015). Evaluators
might locate their story within the population of deep stories. Evaluators grow
up in a particular region, social class, and identify with those in their
vocation. Having an idea of your position helps understand others and the
constellation of perspectives in the evaluation space. An inability or
reluctance to understand other perspectives is a common failure of American
foreign policy. It can damage evaluations.
I’m
not suggesting that all views are relative or equally good. Some views are
better than others because they are more moral and soundly based. An informed
view includes a grasp of other views. A moral view takes into account the
welfare of others. Even in fragmented societies, there may be room for
agreement based on shared deep values. For example, in conducting an evaluation
of environmental education in Lake Charles, I would focus on shared deep
values. I would be unlikely to change their world view, but I might find
agreement about the value of environmental education in specific areas, such as
how pollution affects their children long term. They care deeply about their
children’s future, and they have a sense of fairness. They want to be fair to
their children. The deep value of fairness plays a central role for everyone.
Control for Biases to Enhance Fairness
and Honesty
Being biased
means being influenced by things evaluators should not be influenced by. Biases
might be technical, like sampling error, or social, like racial and sexual
framing, or situational, like conflict of interest, or psychological, like
inappropriate anchoring. Whatever the biases, they can result in distorted
findings. Stakeholders are justified in seeing biased studies as unfair. Taking
care to mitigate bias is critical in conducting fair evaluations. Fortunately,
there are analyses about how to protect against different biases (Scriven,
1976, Shadish, Cook, and Campbell, 2002, Kahneman,
2011, House, 2011).
Focus on Racial and Sexual Framing
Racial framing is
a key mechanism through which racist beliefs are perpetuated. They play a huge
role in American history (Feagin, 2011, DuBois, 1986). The white racial frame
asserts that African Americans are violent, criminal, unintelligent, lazy, and
oversexed (Feagin, 2013, p. 101). Whites are superior. Whites are immersed in
racial framing in childhood and often act on it unconsciously (House, 2017).
The effects are pernicious. Something similar happens with stereotyping
females. The tacit nature of racial and sexual framing makes such biases
extremely difficult to eradicate. Evaluators should look carefully for social
biases in programs and evaluations, including program effects (House, 2017).
They should check their own predispositions, those of colleagues, and expect
them to check theirs.
Be Transparent
Trust is at a
premium in fragmented societies. Transparency engenders trust; lack of
transparency engenders mistrust. In climates of distrust, people imagine bad
things are happening. Evaluators should be transparent about what they’re
doing. In a Denver bilingual education program beset with years of distrust
between the school administration and Latino community, in my role as federal
court monitor, I made clear to each group what I was doing. I was open to
discussion and recommendations from them. I shared the data we collected and
solicited advice as to what to collect next to determine whether the program
was being implemented properly. Over time transparency helped establish trust
in the evaluation and improved trust among groups. Transparency is no panacea,
but it helps.
Implementing the Practices
The purpose of these practices is to enhance the effectiveness of
evaluations and address societal problems. The practices, singly or collectively,
could strengthen many evaluations.
Acting as a moral fiduciary would be most useful in projects where there is a steep hierarchy. Inequalities
in the larger society are manifested in evaluations. This can occur in large or
small, formative or summative evaluations. An early evaluation task would be to
carefully consider the hierarchy. Race, gender, and social class are key. The
criterion for success is how well the evaluation protects and prioritizes the
interests of those least advantaged.
For example, in the “Women Affirming Motherhood” program, two women
developed a program to provide pre and postnatal care to minority mothers (Alkin and Christie, 2019). Later, a large NYC foundation
offered funding if the program became one of several demonstrating interagency cooperation, a foundation goal. There
evolved a steep socioeconomic hierarchy extending from the mothers and infants
to the chair of the foundation. This hierarchy included the mothers and
infants, those working directly with the mothers and infants, the program founders,
the coordinating agencies, the foundation officers, and the foundation board. The
hierarchy itself presented problems of understanding and communication.
In such a hierarchy,
the interests of the mothers and infants might well be neglected. Those in each
position have different perspectives, values, and interests, as well as age, race,
gender, and class differences. Since the program was developing, I proposed a formative
evaluation focused on helping the mothers and infants foremost, those who were least
advantaged. A central evaluation focus was to determine whether they were
getting the services they needed. Even with the best intentions, their welfare
might be overlooked amidst the micropolitics and machinations of a large
endeavor. We have seen the value of patient advocates who help patients in
large hospitals. I envisioned the program founders, mothers, and core workers
as prime audiences for the evaluation since they had most effect on the infants
and most urgent need for information. (For details about how this might play
out, see House, 2019).
Cultivating cognitive empathy can be complementary to any evaluation strategy, large or small, perhaps
more useful in formative studies. There are several ways to accomplish this,
depending on evaluation resources. The most likely path is for the evaluator to
grasp other viewpoints through extensive discussions. Cognitive empathy should
make a difference in how the evaluation is designed and received. If not, the
evaluator isn’t delving deeply enough. Attaining cultural empathy can be built
into the evaluation design by including regular interactions with stakeholders
as on-going data collection in the study.
Focusing on deep stories and deep values provides a sense of direction about what
to look for. What are these people about? What do they want? How do they see
the world? There is a logic to their actions from their perspective.
Some glimpse of this logic is invaluable in designing and carrying out an
effective evaluation. Evaluators don’t need polished studies, such as those constructed
by sociologists like Hochschild (2016), Kramer (2016), and Desmond (2016). Generally,
the more interactions evaluators have with stakeholders, the better the
possibility of understanding.
There are a few deep values that bind us as a community, including
family, fairness, and in-group loyalty, as well as sexual and racial attitudes.
How these are woven into deep stories marks us and leads us to act in certain
ways. Since people share deep values, there is usually room for some cooperation,
even amidst fragmentation.
Controlling for
biases is obligatory for all evaluations. By biases
I mean mistakes that can invalidate the study. If an evaluator draws an
inappropriate sample, employs the wrong statistic, or uses racial or sexual stereotypes,
observers might rightfully claim that such a study is biased, unfair, and even
invalid. Over decades evaluators and others have assembled lists of actions and
conditions that can cause people to arrive at incorrect findings in certain
conditions. We call these biases. Knowledge of biases is core knowledge in
evaluation. All biases have been demonstrated empirically to cause errors when
drawing conclusions.
How transparent
should evaluators be? There should be no secret side deals with various
stakeholders. Even if innocuous, these destroy trust. Explaining the study may
be enough, but if some stakeholders are more interested in particular aspects
of the study, evaluators should accommodate them. There is no need to explain
every detail. Rather, tell them what they need to know and what they believe
they need to know. Interact with them frequently. Keep them informed
periodically. Face to face meetings are most useful. People can “read” how
credible you are through non-verbal cues. Also, you can learn much important
information you never suspected. Transparency establishes trust in times of
distrust.
Connection to Theory
The ethic of the moral
fiduciary fits a long tradition of moral philosophy. Rawls (1971) theory of
justice as fairness changed the dominant utilitarian conception of justice.
With utilitarianism, you could justify fighting inflation by inducing a
recession that forced large numbers of workers out of jobs. They would suffer,
but the larger society benefited--greatest good for the greatest
number—but without regard for how benefits and suffering were distributed
within society. In elaborate arguments, Rawls said this was unfair. We should
attend to those less advantaged as a moral duty.
Later critics of
Rawls said those left out of the decision processes should also have some voice
in making decisions that affect them. They could not always rely on decision
makers to make the right decisions without being included in the discussions.
Deliberative democratic advocates recommended inclusion, discussion, and
deliberation of stakeholders in decision making and evaluation (Gutmann and
Thompson, 1996, House and Howe, 1999).
However, democratic
participation alone doesn’t seem potent enough in a society so unequal.
Evaluators should take stronger actions to protect the interests of those less
advantaged. Acting as moral fiduciaries is one way to help. Instead of balancing
the interests of different stakeholders equally or putting the interests of
sponsors first, evaluators might give priority to those who need help most. The
core value is fairness, fairness in both society and evaluation.
References
Alkin, M. and C. A. Christie. (Eds.). (2017) Theorists’ Models in
Action. New Directions in Evaluation, 163, Fall, 161-72.
Bebchuk, L and Fried, J. (2004). Pay without performance.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Campbell, R., Shaw, J., and Fehler-Cabral. (2015). Shelving justice: The discovery of
thousands of untested rape kits in Detroit. City
and Community, 14:2, June, 151-166.
Cramer, K. (2016). The politics of
resentment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Desmond, M. (2016). Evicted. New York:
Crown.
DuBois, W. E. B. (1986). Writings. Library of America. New York.
Feagin, J. R. (2013). The white racial frame. (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.
Guttmann, A. and Thompson, D. Democracy and
disagreement. Cambridge MA: Belknap.
Hacker, J. S. and Pierson, P. (2020). Let them eat tweets. New York:
Liveright.
Hochschild, A. R. (2016). Strangers in
their own land. New York: New Press.
House, E. R. and Howe, K. R. (1999). Values
in evaluation and social research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
House, E. R. (2011). Conflict of interest and Campbellian validity. New Directions for Evaluation,
2011, 69-80.
House, E. R. (2015). Evaluating: Values,
biases, and practical wisdom Charlotte NC: Information Age.
House, E. R. (2017). Evaluation and the
framing of race. American Journal of Evaluation, 38(2), 167-189.
House, E. R. (2019). Evaluation with a focus
on justice. In M. Alkin and C. A. Christie. (Eds). Theorists’
Models in Action. New Directions
in Evaluation, 163, Fall, 161-72.
House, E. R. (2020). Evaluating in a
fragmented society. Journal of MultiDisciplinary Evaluation. 16(36), 26-36.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and
slow. New York: Farrar, Strauss, Giroux.
Kennedy, D. M. (1999). Freedom from fear.
New York: Oxford University.
Kramer, K, (2016). The politics of
resentment. Chicago: University of Chicago.
Krugman, P. (2017). The gilded age. In
H. Boushey, J.B. DeLong, and M. Steinbaum (Eds). Beyond
Piketty. (2014). Cambridge MA:
Harvard University Press. 60-71.
Lessig, L. (2018). America,
compromised. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Piketty, T. (2014).
Capital in the
twenty-first century. Cambridge
MA: Belknap.
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge MA: Belknap.
Rothstein. B. (2018). How the trust trap
perpetuates inequality. Scientific American. 319, 5, 7.
Saez, E. and Zucman, G. (2020). The
triumph of injustice. NY: W.W. Norton.
Schwandt, T. A. (2019). Post-normal
evaluation? Evaluation. Vol 25(3),
317-329.
Small, M. L. (2019). Rhetoric and social
science in a polarized society. 2019 Spencer Lecture, American Educational
Research Association, Toronto, Canada, May 22.
Scriven, M. (1976). Evaluation bias and its
control. In G. V Glass (Ed.). Evaluation studies review annual. Pp. 119-139. Beverley Hills: Sage.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., and Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs
for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Stiglitz, J. E. (2018). A rigged economy. Scientific
American, 319:5, 57-61.
No comments:
Post a Comment